Wednesday, November 27, 2013

Theory of mind


I first heard the expression "theory of mind" from professor Milind Watve when I was visiting IISER Pune in the middle of last year, shortly before I joined here myself. Somehow I don't recall hearing the expression before that. It is not a theory of how the mind works. Rather, it is an ability - the ability to appreciate that other people have a different mind from one's own. Wikipedia defines it as the "ability to attribute mental states - beliefs, intents, desires, pretending, knowledge, etc. - to oneself and others and to understand that others have beliefs, desires, and intentions that are different from one's own."

It's strange that such an obvious ability should even have a name. After all, there is no "theory of body" - the ability to appreciate that other people have a body different from one's own! If I go for a walk I don't assume everyone else is going to do the same, and if I punch someone in the face (true confession: I have never ever hit anyone in my entire life, yes I know it's shameful ;-) ) I don't assume this action will injure my own face.

And yet, when it comes to the mind, this sort of clarity is not universal. On one hand it appears to be present in birds, but on the other, it's strikingly absent in a whole lot of people I know. So perhaps the term "birdbrain" is a little unfair! But let me first tell you about Milind and the birds (I am recounting last year's talk from memory, so it may not be complete accurate). With the help of undergraduate students, he set up and studied the following situation. A particular species of bird lays its eggs in a nest that's designed like a tunnel dug into a hillside. The eggs hatch inside and the bird enters regularly with food for the chicks. However, the entrance of the nest is concealed and the bird would naturally like to keep it that way, to avoid predators enjoying its offspring for dinner. So it never enters the nest when someone is looking. Instead, when it returns home with tasty worm-flavoured desserts it perches at some distance and waits. Only when it judges no one is looking does it enter the nest.

Here's where the bird's "theory of mind" comes in. If a human being hangs around in a place where she can be seen by the bird and can also see the entrance, the bird will simply wait. Only after the human being goes away will it enter the nest. But now suppose that the human stands in a place where she cannot see the entrance of the nest (e.g. her line of sight is blocked by a tree). And suppose the bird can see her, but can also see that she (the human) does not have a view of the nest. Experiments appear to show that in this situation the bird is confident that there is no danger, and will therefore go ahead and enter.

Good for birds, I have to say. But are human beings equally skilled? Experiments show that children below the age of 4 tend to fail the "false belief task" or "Sally-Anne test", a  standard probe for the presence of theory of mind. Once they cross that age they tend to pass the test. I don't deal much with children just over 4, but most of the adults I know would not do too well. At least, the people I interact with constantly assume that if they know something, you must know the same thing. They assume that if they have a certain intention then you must have the same intention. They expect that if they believe something, then you believe the same thing.

There's a good side to this: people who lack a sound "theory of mind" tend to give away their secrets easily. In my experience, those who constantly describe others as crooked and corrupt tend to be crooked and corrupt themselves. People who are always suspicious of others are often up to some mischief on their own part. They attribute bad motivations to others because they have bad motivations themselves, and lacking a "theory of mind" they do not realise that others may function differently. So, the next time you meet a suspicious person, ask them to do a better job of hiding their own motivations.

Sunday, November 10, 2013

The amazing Julian Schwinger


After a long gap, I'm back to this blog. I don't know if I have any readers left, let alone whether any of them are physicists. On this one occasion I want to point out something that physicists should find fascinating if they didn't already know it (other readers may or may not follow some details, sorry about that).

During the past year I gave a few talks on the discovery of the Higgs particle during the peak periods of public interest (i.e. just after the discovery, and just after the Nobel announcement). In the process I waded through a large number of original papers. The one that surprised me the most is Julian Schwinger's 1957 paper "A theory of the fundamental interactions" published in Annals of Physics 2 (1957), 404-434. At the time Schwinger had already done his path-breaking work on quantum electrodynamics and was, so to say, awaiting a Nobel prize that would finally be given in 1965.

In the 1957 paper Schwinger attempted to create, almost by pure thought, what today we call the Standard Model of fundamental interactions. He did not succeed, lacking many ingredients (Yang-Mills theory was around but he didn't use it  and maybe didn't know of it, concepts like spontaneous symmetry breaking and the Higgs mechanism had not been developed and the right symmetries and multiplet structure were not known). Yet the paper certainly influenced most of the subsequent work and laid down an approach that bore fruit a mere decade later. This fact is widely acknowledged, but the surprise for me was the amazing level of insight in the paper and the sense it conveys of being very far ahead of its time.

The paper starts with a quote from Einstein: "The axiomatic basis of theoretical physics cannot be extracted from experience but must be freely invented". At face value this would infuriate a lot of people. Today it even reads like a sales pitch for string theory! But examine the sentence more carefully and nuances emerge. It is not that physics itself must be freely invented, but rather that theory cannot be extracted directly from experiment, there is an "inventive" stage in between - one that Einstein superbly implemented while converting Michelson and Morley's experiment to the Special Theory of Relativity.

I don't want to summarise Schwinger's entire paper but will just highlight its opening and closing sections. Phrases in italics are direct quotes from the paper. The very first paragraph is a manifesto of sorts. Here Schwinger proposes the following principles:

(i) Spins 0, 1/2 and 1 are fundamental in nature, having exceptional simplicity and allowing for the construction of "unique" theories (the sense in which he intends "unique" is not made precise).

(ii) "If the spin values are thus limited, the origin of the diversity of known particles must be sought in internal degrees of freedom"

(iii) "The various intrinsic degrees of freedom are dynamically exhibited by specific interactions, each with its characteristic symmetry properties..."

(iv) "...the final effect of interactions with successively lower symmetry is to produce a spectrum of physically distinct particles from initially degenerate states"

(v) If you know what the Higgs mechanism is, be prepared to be amazed by this line: "Thus we attempt to relate the observed masses to the same couplings responsible for the production and interaction of these particles."

Today we know that the mass of each particle is proportional to how strongly the Higgs particle couples to it. But how on earth did Schwinger guess this, or something that sounds like it? I find this amazing.

The paper develops the idea that an intermediate vector boson (which he calls Z, though in today's language it is really the W) should be responsible for weak interactions, and concludes modestly as follows:

"What has been presented here is an attempt to elaborate a complete dynamical theory of the elementary particles from a few general concepts. Such a connected series of speculations can be of value if it provides a convenient frame of reference in seeking a more coherent account of natural phenomena."

If by "frame of reference" he meant "a way of thinking about things" then his hope was more than fulfilled: the intermediate vector boson idea permeated the work of many including Englert-Brout and Higgs, Glashow and of course Weinberg who in 1967 essentially wrote down the correct theory.